Custom Search

Sunday, 10 August 2008

Stress Can Be External And Internal

Your Ad Here Stress seems to be catch all term for almost anything that makes us feel enough pressure to be anxious, from truly serious to be seemingly trivial. Strictly speaking stress is defined as reaction to a physical, mental or mental or emotional stimulus that upset the body’s natural balance this could apply to most of us some of the time, or even all of the time.

Our body must have evolved to cope with stress, but the peculiar, possibly unprecedented, kinds of stress that modern life inflicts on us do seems to have adverse consequences on the body, in both the short and the long term. Most people recognize some things that cause stress in their life, such as work load, overdue bills, traffic, traffic wardens ect... But some of the things people don’t recognize as inducers are:

  • Lack of sleep
  • Excessive alcohol
  • Smoking
  • Blood sugar imbalance
  • Nutrient deficiencies
  • Food allergies and intolerance
  • Medicines and hormones imbalance
  • A poorly function bowel leading to toxic waste build up

Work Related Stress

Depression, anxiety, drug or alcohol abuse can be symptoms of stress.
Stress is good up to a point . It motivates and keeps people attentive. But clearly, when the economy is down . . . it is something to which people react to."

Layne Prest, director of behavioral medicine and the medical family therapy program at the Nebraska Medical Center, said he, too, has seen an increase in the number of people seeking help for symptoms of stress. Forty percent of his clients at the One World Medical Clinic, who are primarily Latino, have stress-related symptoms. A declining economy hits blue-collar workers especially hard, he said. "They have to work two to three jobs to make ends meet."

Also is seeing more Caucasians at One World Medical Clinic who are seeking free or reduced-price health care because they have lost or don't have insurance coverage. Sometimes work-related stress is more subtle than losing a job or fearing the loss of a job. Or the fear and worry about family situations that keep them from performing up to what they perceive as higher standards at work.

Tips For Stress

  • "Take a step back and be as realistic as they can.
  • "People should consider if their fears are realistic," "Do they feel things are out of control, are they overwhelmed? Where can they make changes? Generally, don't try to control things you can't."

In addition to the economic downturn, there always are people who feel trapped in a job or with a manager they don't like. Whether it's a job or a bad marriage, relationships can be "extraordinarily stressful, too," he said. "People recommend finding another job, but that is not always easy to do, and moving is stressful, too. It is still putting someone out of a routine."

Friends and family members can be helpful sounding boards, but sometimes the situation calls for professional help from an independent, trained observer. "If people are ruminating about it (their situation) over and over and don't have an opportunity to get away from it, they should seek some professional evaluation," including using employee assistance programs at work.

"They should keep their resume up-to-date and keep their employment network in the picture," Meanwhile, people should keep focused on their lives and careers.
In a situation were a company is considering downsizing, if workers start looking at each other as competitors, relationships can break down. That can actually hurt a person's performance and his or her chances of success. The unknown can cause a lot of worry, and workers aren't always knowledgeable about things going on in a company.
Workers should find out as much as they can about their companies, to know whether fears about their and their companies' futures are warranted. Publicly held companies are required by law to provide substantial information,

Stress is an external and internal experience. You can reduce your external stimuli by changing certain elements in your lives such as changing jobs or not driving to work. However, a lot of stress is actually how you perceive things internally. Do you let things get to you? Or do you let things do? This is something that can change your relationship with stress significantly. For those who are not able to do this themselves, consider contacting a cognitive behavioral therapist. You may also consider 300 breakthrough stress relief tips!
  • Set aside 15 minutes every day for yourself to think, dream and not be pressurized by anything with in your daily plans. Note: watching television is not relaxing as it is designed to keep you alert and awake.
  • Avoid a diet that puts further stress on your adrenals. This include limiting alcohol to seven unit’s a week, stopping caffeine intake- this put quite a strain on the adrenals-and all diet and soft drinks
  • Stop consuming all fast releasing sugars as they create a state of stress in the body, stimulating the release of cortical. This means avoiding white bread and pasta, sweat breakfast cereals, and anything else that added sugar to it. Substitute these with complex carbohydrates that help to stabilize blood sugar levels, e.g. brown rice, wholemeal bread and pasta, oats, and quinoa.
  • Avoid processed foods, food products that contain chemical additives, and all fried foods as they put and additional stress on the body.
  • Eat a well- balanced diet with lots of fruit and vegetables. If you are in a stresses state, you may need extra protein so go for protein-rich foods such as oily fish and eggs( or vegetable proteins if you don’t eat animal products)
  • Avoid eating under stressful conditions. If you eat too quickly or under stress, you will not absorb your nutrients and your digestive system will not work well. Eat in a relaxed environment to ensure there is sufficient blood flow and effective digestion.
  • Exercise three times a week for 20 or 30 minutes. It is great for reducing anxiety and nervousness and for elevating your brain chemicals to make you feel good



Friday, 8 August 2008

Alienation Test

Your Ad Here

Early Theories

There are two early cognitive social theories, those of Bandura and Mischel. Bandura pioneered
the study of observational learning (or vicarious conditioning). He believed that, rather than operating in a mechanistic way, reinforcement provides information about future reinforcement. Such information can be gleaned by watching models' behavior rather than by behaving in a particular way and experiencing the consequences oneself. Note how this definition of reinforcement differs from that of Skinner, for whom one had to experience reinforcement personally to increase a target behavior. Note also that for Bandura, thinking is not an irrelevant activity that occurs within a "black box," but rather is an important object of study in its own right.

A number of points distinguish the cognitive social approach from other approaches, including the following.

    Cognitive Focus--Remember Skinner and the "black box"? This approach is nothing like that. For example, memory of past reinforcements is an important variable mediating stimulus and response.

    Social-Interpersonal Focus--Remember Skinner and the generalization from pigeons and rats to all organisms (including humans)? This approach is nothing like that. Instead, the focus is on human behavior in particular situations, and the most important situational variable is other people.

    Belief in Human Freedom and Choice--Remember Skinner and the call to go "beyond freedom and dignity" to scientific understanding and control of human behavior? This approach is nothing like that. Instead, there is a belief in human choice from a number of possible behaviors. The environment does not only influence the person--the person also influences the environment. (This is called reciprocal determination.)

Five kinds of cognitive social learning variables can be distinguished (Mischel, 1993):
    Encoding Strategies: How do you see it?
    Expectancies: What will happen?
    Values: What is it worth? What are your goals?
    Plans: How can you achieve it?
    Competencies: What can you do?

Although these cognitive variables obviously have an impact on social relations, a question of keen interest is whether they can be reduced to general intelligence. For example, are competencies part of the "positive manifold"--that is, are they positively correlated with all other cognitive abilities? If so, then perhaps theorists would be better off focusing on general intelligence rather than on each individual cognitive social learning variable.

Cognition and Emotion

Higgins' Theory

Higgins (1987) proposed that each person has multiple mental representations of the self, and that a discrepancy between any pair of these representations has emotional consequences. The three big categories of representations are actual, ideal, and ought self. The actual self is who one really is. The ideal self is who one would like to be. The ought self is who one feels it is one's duty to be.

The actual, ideal, and ought selves can be further divided according to whether they are held by oneself or by others. For example, there is an actual/own self, an ideal/own self, and an ideal/other self. An actual/own:ideal/own discrepancy (for example) results in dejection, whereas an actual/own:ideal/other discrepancy results in shame.

Higgins' (1987) theory is an elaborate way of subdividing personality. Higgins' division of personality into six components might be compared to Freud's division into three (id, ego, and superego).

Seligman's Theory

Seligman's theory of learned helplessness was originally applied to dogs tested in a shuttle box with a divider separating two sides. Dogs who were shocked on one side eventually jumped over to the other, and, finding that they were not shocked there, learned the jumping response. However, dogs who were initially shocked uncontrollably failed to learn the jumping response, even if they did happen to jump over randomly once or twice.

The phenomenon of learned helplessness bears much in common with depression in humans. The theory was reformulated (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978) in order to take account of explanatory style--that is, the way people explain negative events to themselves. People who have a pessimistic explanatory style explain negative events as stable, global, and internal. Such people are hypothesized to be more predisposed to depression than people with an optimistic explanatory style, who explain negative events as unstable, specific, and external.

The reformulated learned helplessness model (Abramson et al., 1978) bears a striking similarity to the negative cognitive triad in Beck's cognitive theory of depression (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979).

Abramson, Seligman, and Teasdale (1978)

Beck, Rush, Shaw, and Emery (1979)

internal attributionsnegative thoughts about the self
global attributions negative thoughts about the world
stable attributionsnegative thoughts about the future


Alienation Test

"The statements below concern how you feel in emotionally intimate relationships. We are interested in how you generally experience relationships, not just in what is happening in a current relationship.... When you have answered all the questions, the web page will determine your attachment style." By R. Chris Fraley.

Please record how you feel about each sentence as follows:

5 -- I agree strongly
4 -- I agree
3 -- I am neutral, or I don't know
2 -- I disagree
1 -- I disagree strongly

[For a printable, self-scoring version of this test, click here]

1. I don't understand the way people behave nowadays.
2. I don't want what most people seem to want.
3. The future of mankind looks pretty hopeless.
4. Most people act as if the end justifies the means.
5. I don't get much satisfaction from my work (or school work).
6. It's a lonely life for more and more people nowadays.
7. Things don't make much sense to me anymore.
8. My values are different from society's values.
9. There is little room for personal choice anymore.
10. There just aren't any definite rules to live by today.
11. I wish I could feel more involved in my job (or school work).
12. I wish people would be a lot kinder than they are.
13. I feel confused about the world a lot.
14. Most people don't have the same priorities that I do.
15. You can only get ahead if you get some lucky breaks.
16. It seems that right and wrong are pretty ambivalent nowadays.
17. Sometimes I just feel like a robot at work (or school).
18. Sometimes I feel all alone in the world.
19. I don't know what the purpose of life is anymore.
20. I don't identify with my culture's values.
21. There are so many decisions to make that I could just scream.
22. It seems as if you have to play dirty to win.
23. I don't have much opportunity to be creative.
24. I don't get to go out with friends much anymore.
25. Life has become less and less meaningful to me.
26. Everybody seems to have a different idea of success than I have.
27. It is (or would be) scary to be responsible for a child nowadays.
28. It often seems that it's the nice people who lose.
29. It's frustrating if you really care about the quality of your work.
30. I don't see my family as much as I'd like to.




Your Ad Here




Tuesday, 5 August 2008

Household frictions

Your Ad Here
Exploring Contemporary Psychology: Social Egocentrism

In understanding how relationships are formed and maintained, one important issue concerns the frictions that can develop in a relationship, including the frictions that emerge as part of day-to-day living: "Why don't you ever do the dishes — why is it always me?" "Why is it that I'm usually the one who takes out the garbage; why can't you do your fair share?" Or: "Why is it that I'm always the one who reaches out to end our disagreements? Don't you know the words, ‘I'm sorry'?"

These frictions can arise because sometimes responsibilities are inequitably distributed in a relationship, and this can, of course, be a source of stress. Other factors can also contribute to these frictions: Sometimes, people in a relationship have a view of who-does-what that's shaped by self-flattery or self-service. These forces can lead someone to inflate their estimates of how much they contribute to the maintenance of the household, or the relationship itself. This inflated sense of their own contribution then leads to a perceived imbalance, and, of course, to stresses in the relationship.

But another effect also contributes to these frictions: Thinking, they argued that people often judge frequency by trying to think of relevant cases, and gauging how easily these come to mind. Are more of your friends male or female? To find out, you might try to think of male friends and female friends. If a list of men comes quickly to mind, this is an indication that most of your friends are males; if a list of women comes to your thoughts more easily, this would suggest the opposite conclusion.



How does this apply to the frictions we have described? When you take out the garbage, you obviously are aware of this event; when your house mate takes out the garbage, you may not even be around. Likewise, when you reach out to end an argument, this is often a difficult step as you swallow your anger and struggle to submerge your own feelings for the good of the relationship. That sort of thing should be well-remembered, and will probably be better remembered than the occasions in which it's your partner who backs down (because in those cases, you do see their conciliatory gesture, but don't see the thought process that led up to it). For all these reasons, you'll end up with a better memory for your own actions than your housemate's. This will lead to a bias in the sorts of cases that come to mind when you think about taking out the garbage, or settling fights, and this in turn will produce a bias in assessed frequency. Because each of us is better able to remember our own actions, we are likely to overestimate the frequency of our own actions, relative to others.

Evidence for these claims comes from a study comparing the "egocentric bias" (claiming more than your share of the credit) for good deeds like taking out the garbage, and for bad deeds like provoking fights, or leaving the kitchen a mess. It turns out that degree of egocentric bias is the same for the good deeds and the bad (Ross & Siccoly, 1979). This is what we might expect on grounds of memory bias, but not what we'd expect if the bias comes out of vain self-flattery. (In that case, people would take too much credit for the good deeds, but too little credit for the bad!) Such evidence argues that memory availability does play a role in producing frictions, and reminds us that our account of social relationships must include the perceptions and memories that influence us as we participate in those relationships!






Sunday, 3 August 2008

Bowlby Attachment

Your Ad Here The theory of attachment was originally developed by John Bowlby (1907 - 1990), a British psychoanalyst who was attempting to understand the intense distress experienced by infants who had been separated from their parents. Bowlby observed that separated infants would go to extraordinary lengths (e.g., crying, clinging, frantically searching) to either prevent separation from their parents or to reestablish proximity to a missing parent. At the time, psychoanalytic writers held that these expressions were manifestations of immature defense mechanisms that were operating to repress emotional pain, but Bowlby noted that such expressions are common to a wide variety of mammalian species, and speculated that these behaviors may serve an evolutionary function.

Drawing on ethological theory, Bowlby postulated that these attachment behaviors, such as crying and searching, were adaptive responses to separation from with a primary attachment figure--someone who provides support, protection, and care. Because human infants, like other mammalian infants, cannot feed or protect themselves, they are dependent upon the care and protection of "older and wiser" adults. Bowlby argued that, over the course of evolutionary history, infants who were able to maintain proximity to an attachment figure (i.e., by looking cute or by expressing in attachment behaviors) would be more likely to survive to a reproductive age. According to Bowlby attachment, a motivational-control system, what he called the attachment behavioral system, was gradually "designed" by natural selection to regulate proximity to an attachment figure.


Background: Bowlby Attachment theories

The attachment behavior system is an important concept in attachment theory because it provides the conceptual linkage between ethological models of human development and modern theories on emotion regulation and personality. According to Bowlby, the attachment system essentially "asks" the following fundamental question: Is the attachment figure nearby, accessible, and attentive? If the child perceives the answer to this question to be "yes," he or she feels loved, secure, and confident, and, behaviorally, is likely to explore his or her environment, play with others, and be sociable. If, however, the child perceives the answer to this question to be "no," the child experiences anxiety and, behaviorally, is likely to exhibit attachment behaviors ranging from simple visual searching on the low extreme to active following and vocal signaling on the other (see Figure 1). These behaviors continue until either the child is able to reestablish a desirable level of physical or psychological proximity to the attachment figure, or until the child "wears down," as may happen in the context of a prolonged separation or loss. In such cases or helplessness, Bowlby believed the child experiences despair and depression.


Individual Differences in Infant Attachment Patterns

Although Bowlby believed that the basic dynamics described above captured the normative dynamics of the attachment behavioral system, he recognized that there are individual differences in the way children appraise the accessibility of the attachment figure and how they regulate their attachment behavior in response to a threat. However, it wasn't until his colleague, Mary Ainsworth, began to systematically study infant-parent separations that a formal understanding of these individual differences was articulated. Ainsworth and her students developed a technique called the strange situation--a laboratory paradigm for studying infant-parent attachment. In the strange situation, 12-month-old infants and their parents are brought to the laboratory and, systematically, separated and reunited. In the strange situation, most children (i.e., about 60%) behave in the way implied by Bowlby's "normative" theory. They become upset when the parent leaves the room, but, when he or she returns, they actively seek the parent and are easily comforted by him or her. Children who exhibit this pattern of behavior are often called secure. Other children (about 20% or less) are ill-at-ease initially, and, upon separation, become extremely distressed. Importantly, when reunited with their parents, these children have a difficult time being soothed, and often exhibit conflicting behaviors that suggest they want to be comforted, but that they also want to "punish" the parent for leaving. These children are often called anxious-resistant. The third pattern of attachment that Ainsworth and her colleagues documented is called avoidant. Avoidant children (about 20%) don't appear too distressed by the separation, and, upon reunion, actively avoid seeking contact with their parent, sometimes turning their attention to play objects on




the laboratory floor. Ainsworth's work was important for at least three reasons. First, she provided one of the first empirical demonstrations of how attachment behavior is patterned in both safe and frightening contexts. Second, she provided the first empirical taxonomy of individual differences in infant attachment patterns. According to her research, at least three types of children exist: those who are secure in their relationship with their parents, those who are anxious-resistant, and those who are anxious-avoidant. Finally, she demonstrated that these individual differences were correlated with infant-parent interactions in the home during the first year of life. Children who appear secure in the strange situation, for example, tend to have parents who are responsive to their needs. Children who appear insecure in the strange situation (i.e., anxious-resistant or avoidant) often have parents who are insensitive to their needs, or inconsistent or rejecting in the care they provide.

Adult Romantic Relationships

Although Bowlby was primarily focused on understanding the nature of the infant-caregiver relationship, he believed that attachment characterized human experience from "the cradle to the grave." It was not until the mid-1980's, however, that researchers began to take seriously the possibility that attachment processes may play out in adulthood. Hazan and Shaver (1987) were two of the first researchers to explore Bowlby's ideas in the context of romantic relationships. According to Hazan and Shaver, the emotional bond that develops between adult romantic partners is partly a function of the same motivational system--the attachment behavioral system--that gives rise to the emotional bond between infants and their caregivers. Hazan and Shaver noted that infants and caregivers and adult romantic partners share the following features:

  • both feel safe when the other is nearby and responsive
  • both engage in close, intimate, bodily contact
  • both feel insecure when the other is inaccessible
  • both share discoveries with one another
  • both play with one another's facial features and exhibit a mutual fascination and preoccupation with one another
  • both engage in "baby talk"

On the basis of these parallels, Hazan and Shaver argued that adult romantic relationships, like infant-caregiver relationships, are attachments, and that romantic love is a property of the attachment behavioral system, as well as the motivational systems that give rise to caregiving and sexuality.

Three Implications of Adult Attachment Theory

The idea that romantic relationships may be attachment relationships has had a profound influence on modern research on close relationships. There are at least three critical implications of this idea. First, if adult romantic relationships are attachment relationships, then we should observe the same kinds of individual differences in adult relationships that Ainsworth observed in infant-caregiver relationships. We may expect some adults, for example, to be secure in their relationships--to feel confident that their partners will be there for them when needed, and open to depending on others and having others depend on them. We should expect other adults, in contrast, to be insecure in their relationships. For example, some insecure adults may be anxious-resistant: they worry that others may not love them completely, and be easily frustrated or angered when their attachment needs go unmet. Others may be avoidant: they may appear not to care too much about close relationships, and may prefer not to be too dependent upon other people or to have others be too dependent upon them.


Second, if adult romantic relationships are attachment relationships, then the way adult relationships "work" should be similar to the way infant-caregiver relationships work. In other words, the same kinds of factors that facilitate exploration in children (i.e., having a responsive caregiver) should facilitate exploration among adults (i.e., having a responsive partner). The kinds of things that make an attachment figure "desirable" for infants (i.e., responsiveness, availability) are the kinds of factors we should find desirable in adult romantic partners. Importantly, individual differences in attachment should influence relational and personal functioning in adulthood in the same way they do in childhood.

Third, whether an adult is secure or insecure in his or her adult relationships may be a partial reflection of his or her attachment experiences in early childhood. Bowlby believed that the mental representations or working models (i.e., expectations, beliefs, "rules" or "scripts" for behaving and thinking) that a child holds regarding relationships are a function of his or her caregiving experiences. For example, a secure child tends to believe that others will be there for him or her because previous experiences have led him or her to this conclusion. Once a child has developed such expectations, he or she will tend to seek out relational experiences that are consistent with those expectations and perceive others in a way that is colored by those beliefs. According to Bowlby, this kind of process should promote continuity in attachment patterns over the life course, although it is possible that a person's attachment pattern will change if his or her relational experiences are inconsistent with his or her expectations. In short, if we assume that adult relationships are attachment relationships, it is possible that children who are secure as children will grow up to be secure in their romantic relationships.

In the sections below I briefly address these three implications in light of early and contemporary research on adult attachment.

Do We Observe the Same Kinds of Attachment Patterns Among Adults that We Observe Among Children?

The earliest research on adult attachment involved studying the association between individual differences in adult attachment and the way people think about their relationships and their memories for what their relationships with their parents are like. Hazan and Shaver (1987) developed a simple questionnaire to measure these individual differences. (These individual differences are often referred to as attachment styles, attachment patterns, attachment orientations, or differences in the organization of the attachment system.) In short, Hazan and Shaver asked research subjects to read the three paragraphs listed below, and indicate which paragraph best characterized the way they think, feel, and behave in close relationships:

A. I am somewhat uncomfortable being close to others; I find it difficult to trust them completely, difficult to allow myself to depend on them. I am nervous when anyone gets too close, and often, others want me to be more intimate than I feel comfortable being.

B. I find it relatively easy to get close to others and

am comfortable depending on them and having them depend on me. I don't worry about being abandoned or about someone getting too close to me.

C. I find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would like. I often worry that my partner doesn't really love me or won't want to stay with me. I want to get very close to my partner, and this sometimes scares people away.

Based on this three category measure, Hazan and Shaver found that the distribution of categories was similar to that observed in infancy. In other words, about 60% of adults classified themselves as secure (paragraph B), about 20% described

themselves as avoidant (paragraph A), and about 20% described themselves as anxious-resistant (paragraph C). Although this measure served as a useful way to study the association between attachment styles and relationship functioning, it didn't allow a full test of the hypothesis that the same kinds of individual differences observed in infants might be manifest among adults. (In many ways, the Hazan and Shaver measure assumed this to be true.) Subsequent research has explored this hypothesis in a variety of ways. For example, Kelly Brennan and her colleagues collected a number of statements (e.g., "I believe that others will be there for me when I need them") and studied the way these statements "hang together" statistically (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). Brennan's findings suggested that there are two fundamental dimensions with respect to adult attachment patterns (see Figure 2). One critical variable has been labeled attachment-related anxiety. People who score high on this variable tend to worry whether their partner is available, responsive, attentive, etc. People who score on the low end of this variable are more secure in the perceived responsiveness of their partners. The other critical variable is called attachment-related avoidance. People on the high end of this dimension prefer not to rely on others or open up to others.




People on the high end of this dimension prefer not to rely on others or open up to others. People on the low end of this dimension are more comfortable being intimate with others and are more secure depending upon and having others depend upon them. A prototypical secure adult is low on both of these dimensions.

Brennan's findings are critical because recent analyses of the statistical patterning of behavior among infants in the strange situation reveal two functionally similar dimensions: one that captures variability in the anxiety and resistance of the child and another that captures variability in the child's willingness to use the parent as a safe haven for support (see Fraley & Spieker, 2003a, 2003b). Functionally, these dimensions are similar to the two-dimensions uncovered among adults, suggesting that similar patterns of attachment exist at different points in the life span.

In light of Brennan's findings, as well as taxometric research published by Fraley and Waller (1998), most researchers currently conceptualize and measure individual differences in attachment dimensionally rather than categorically. The most popular measures of adult attachment style are Brennan, Clark, and Shaver's (1998) ECR and Fraley, Waller, and Brennan's (2000) ECR-R--a revised version of the ECR. [Click here to take an on-line quiz designed to determine your attachment style based on these two dimensions.] Both of these self-report instruments provide continuous scores on the two dimensions of attachment-related anxiety and avoidance.

Do Adult Romantic Relationships "Work" in the Same Way that Infant-Caregiver Relationships Work?

There is now an increasing amount of research that suggests that adult romantic relationships function in the same ways as infant-caregiver relationships, with some noteworthy exceptions, of course. Naturalistic research on adults separating from their partners at an airport demonstrated that behaviors indicative of attachment-related protest and caregiving were evident, and that the regulation of these behaviors was associated with attachment style (Fraley & Shaver, 1998). For example, while separating couples generally showed more attachment behavior than nonseparating couples, highly avoidant adults showed much less attachment behavior than less avoidant adults. In the sections below I discuss some of the parallels that have been discovered between the way that infant-caregiver relationships and adult romantic relationships function.

Partner selection

Cross-cultural studies suggest that the secure pattern of attachment in infancy is universally considered the most desirable pattern by mothers (see van IJzendoorn & Sagi, 1999). For obvious reasons there is no similar study asking infants if they would prefer a security-inducing attachment figure. Adults seeking long-term relationships identify responsive caregiving qualities, such as attentiveness, warmth, and sensitivity, as most "attractive" in potential dating partners (Zeifman & Hazan, 1997). Despite the attractiveness of secure qualities, however, not all adults are paired with secure partners. Some evidence suggests that people end up in relationships with partners who confirm their existing beliefs about attachment relationships (Frazier et al., 1997).

Secure base and safe haven behavior

In infancy, secure infants tend to be the most well adjusted, in the sense that they are relatively resilient, they get along with their peers and are well liked. Similar kinds of patterns have emerged in research on adult attachment. Overall, secure adults tend to be more satisfied in their relationships than insecure adults. Their relationships are characterized by greater longevity, trust, commitment, and interdependence (e.g., Feeney, Noller, & Callan, 1994), and they are more likely to use romantic partners as a secure base from which to explore the world (e.g., Fraley & Davis, 1997). A large proportion of research on adult attachment has been devoted to uncovering the behavioral and psychological mechanisms that promote security and secure base behavior in adults. There have been two major discoveries thus far. First and in accordance with attachment theory, secure adults are more likely than insecure adults to seek support from their partners when distressed. Furthermore, they are more likely to provide support to their distressed partners (e.g., Simpson et al., 1992). Second, the attributions that insecure individuals make concerning their partner's behavior during and following relational conflicts exacerbate, rather than alleviate, their insecurities (e.g., Simpson et al., 1996).




Avoidant Attachment and Defense Mechanisms

According to attachment theory, children differ in the kinds of strategies they adopt to regulate attachment-related anxiety. Following a separation and reunion, for example, some insecure children approach their parents, but with ambivalence and resistance, whereas others withdraw from their parents, apparently minimizing attachment-related feelings and behavior. One of the big questions in the study of infant attachment is whether children who withdraw from their parents--avoidant children--are truly less distressed or whether their defensive behavior is a cover-up for their true feelings of vulnerability. Research that has measured the attentional capacity of children, heart rate, or stress hormone levels suggests that avoidant children are distressed by the separation despite the fact that they come across in a cool, defensive manner.

Recent research on adult attachment has revealed some interesting complexities concerning the relationships between avoidance and defense. Although some avoidant adults, often called fearfully-avoidant adults, are poorly adjusted despite their defensive nature, others, often called dismissing-avoidant adults, are able to use defensive strategies in an adaptive way. For example, in an experimental task in which adults were instructed to discuss losing their partner, Fraley and Shaver (1997) found that dismissing individuals (i.e., individuals who are high on the dimension of attachment-related avoidance but low on the dimension of attachment-related anxiety) were just as physiologically distressed (as assessed by skin conductance measures) as other individuals. When instructed to suppress their thoughts and feelings, however, dismissing individuals were able to do so effectively. That is, they could deactivate their physiological arousal to some degree and minimize the attention they paid to attachment-related thoughts. Fearfully-avoidant individuals were not as successful in suppressing their emotions.

Are Attachment Patterns Stable from Infancy to Adulthood?

Perhaps the most provocative and controversial implication of adult attachment theory is that a person's attachment style as an adult is shaped by his or her interactions with parental attachment figures. Although the idea that early attachment experiences might have an influence on attachment style in romantic relationships is relatively uncontroversial, hypotheses about the source and degree of overlap between the two kinds of attachment orientations have been controversial.

There are at least two issues involved in considering the question of stability: (a) How much similarity is there between the security people experience with different people in their lives (e.g., mothers, fathers, romantic partners)? and (b) With respect to any one of these relationships, how stable is security over time?

With respect to this first issue, it appears that there is a modest degree of overlap between how secure people feel with their mothers, for example, and how secure they feel with their romantic partners. Fraley, for example, collected self-report measures of one's current attachment style with a significant parental figure and a current romantic partner and found correlations ranging between approximately .20 to .50 (i.e., small to moderate) between the two kinds of attachment relationships. [Click here to take an on-line quiz designed to assess the similarity between your attachment styles with different people in your life.]

With respect to the second issue, the stability of one's attachment to one's parents appears to be equal to a correlation of about .25 to .39 (Fraley, 2002). There is only one longitudinal study of which we are aware that assessed the link between security at age 1 in the strange situation and security of the same people 20 years later in their adult romantic relationships. This unpublished study uncovered a correlation of .17 between these two variables (Steele, Waters, Crowell, & Treboux, 1998).

The association between early attachment experiences and adult attachment styles has also been examined in retrospective studies. Hazan and Shaver (1987) found that adults who were secure in their romantic relationships were more likely to recall their childhood relationships with parents as being affectionate, caring, and accepting (see also Feeney & Noller, 1990).

Based on these kinds of studies, it seems likely that attachment styles in the child-parent domain and attachment styles in the romantic relationship domain are only moderately related at best. What are the implications of such findings for adult attachment theory? According to some writers, the most important proposition of the theory is that the attachment system, a system originally adapted for the ecology of infancy, continues to influence behavior, thought, and feeling in adulthood (see Fraley & Shaver, 2000). This proposition may hold regardless of whether individual differences in the way the system is organized remain stable over a decade or more, and stable across different kinds of intimate relationships.

Although the social and cognitive mechanisms invoked by attachment theorists imply that stability in attachment style may be the rule rather than the exception, these basic mechanisms can predict either long-run continuity or discontinuity, depending on the precise ways in which they are conceptualized (Fraley, 2002). Fraley (2002) discussed two models of continuity derived from attachment theory that make different predictions about long-term continuity even though they were derived from the same basic theoretical principles. Each model assumes that individual differences in attachment representations are shaped by variation in experiences with caregivers in early childhood, and that, in turn, these early representations shape the quality of the individual's subsequent attachment experiences. However, one model assumes that existing representations are updated and revised in light of new experiences such that older representations are eventually "overwritten." Mathematical analyses revealed that this model predicts that the long-term stability of individual differences will approach zero. The second model is similar to the first, but makes the additional assumption that representational models developed in the first year of life are preserved (i.e., they are not overwritten) and continue to influence relational behavior throughout the life course. Analyses of this model revealed that long-term stability can approach a non-zero limiting value. The important point here is that the principles of attachment theory can be used to derive developmental models that make strikingly different predictions about the long-term stability of individual differences. In light of this finding, the existence of long-term stability of individual differences should be considered an empirical question rather than an assumption of the theory.






Saturday, 2 August 2008

How People

Your Ad Here
Perceive Oneself

We have a concept of our own selves. What we are really like and why we do what we do? And how people form self concept? ‘’ I am a certain kind of person with such capacities, value beliefs, and attitudes behavior’’, even if we sometimes sugar coat those capacities with a layer of self-serving bias. But how do such human concept arise in the first place.

Human concept

One crucial element is some reference to other people. It is obvious that there can be no full-fledged ‘’I’’ without a ‘’you’’ or a ‘’they’’, for the human concept is a undoubtedly social concept. According to many authors, the child begins to see herself through the eyes of the important figures in her world and thus acquires the idea that she is a person. As the social interactions become more complex, more and more details are added to the self picture. In effect, the child sees herself through the mirror of the opinions and expectations of those others (mothers, fathers, siblings, friends) who matter to her. Her later behavior cannot help but be shaped by this early self theory‘’looking glass self’’. example of such birth, roles defined by race, gender, ethnicity, and so on.







Self perception and attribution

According to the self theory ‘’looking glass theory’’. we learn who we are by finding out through others, by noting how they treat us, how they react to us, and what they expect from us. But isn’t there a more direct methods? Can’t we discover who we are and what we feel simply by observing ourselves? According to some authors the answer is no. In their view, our self concept are attained through an attribution process no different from the one we use to form conception of other people. The advocates of this self perception theory maintain that, contrary to common sense belief, we do no know our own self directly. In their view, self concept ( self knowledge) can only be achieved indirectly, through the same attempts to find consistencies, discount irrelevancies, and interpret observations that help us to understand other people.


In the movie Donnie Brasco, an undercover FBI agent infiltrate the mob. As his involvement deepens, he grows uncertain of his own allegiance

One line of evidence concerns the relation between attitudes and behavior. Common sense argues that attitudes cause behavior, that our own attitudes behavior actions stem from our feeling and our values beliefs. To some extends, this is undoubtedly true. Those in favor of a strong military are likely to join a rally demanding cuts in the defense budget. But under some circumstances, the cause and effect relation is reverse. Some times our feelings or value beliefs are the results of our actions.


A demonstration comes from foot-in-the-door technique, originally perfected by traveling sale men. In one study, suburban homeowners were asked to comply with an innocuous request, to put a three inch square sign advocating auto safety in a window of their homes. Two weeks later, another experimenter came to visit those homeowners who had agreed to display the small sign. This time they were asked to grant a much greater request, to permit the installation of an enormous billboard on their front lawns, proclaiming ‘’Drive Carefully’’ in huge letters while obstructing most of the house. The results showed that agreement depended upon prior agreement. Once having complied with the first, small request, the homeowners were much more likely to give in to the greater one.


The foot-in-the-door and the environment
The foot-in-the-door effect can start at an early age. The photo shows young children induced to do their bit for environment. Whether the cans they collect now make much of a difference matters less than that those acts are likely to lead to greater efforts in the future, as the children come to think of themselves as environmentalist.





One interpretation of this and similar findings is a change in self perception. Having agreed to put small sign, the homeowners now thought of themselves as active citizens involved in a public issue. Since no one forced them to put up the sign, they attributed their action to their own convictions. Given that they now thought of themselves as active, convinced, and involved, they were ready to play the part on a larger scale. Fortunately for their less involved neighbors, the billboard was in fact never installed after all, the request was only part of an experiment. But in real life we may not be let off so easily. The foot-in-the-door approach is a common device for persuading the initial uncommitted; it can be used to peddle encyclopedias or harden political convictions. Extremist political movements generally do not demand violent actions from newcomers. They begin with small request like signing a petition or giving a distinctive salute. But these may lead to a change self perception that ultimately may ready the person for more drastic acts

This line of argument may have some bearing on our understanding of how social systems function. The social world cast people in different roles that prescribe particular sets of behaviors; representatives of labor and management will obviously take different positions at the bargaining table. But the roles determine attitudes behavior. If one acts like a union representative, one starts to feel like one. The same holds for the corporate executive. This point has been verified in a study of factory workers both before and after they were elected union steward or promoted to foreman. As one might have expected, the newly elected union stewards become more pro-union; the newly promoted foreman become more pro-management.






Wednesday, 30 July 2008

What is Today

Your Ad Here
Problems

The pressure of having to solve a problem may be overwhelming. Because we must search for a solution to a problem we have solved before. This search is far from random; it proceeds with constant reference to two anchors: the current situation on the one hand, and the goal on the other hand. Consider a taxi driver who is trying to choose the best route to the airport. His thoughts are guided both by his current location and by thoughts about his destination. Perhaps the taxi is close to the freeway, but this isn’t enough to direct the driver toward the highway’s on ramp. Instead, he is likely to ask ‘’ will this path bring me where I want to go? If the highway heads in the wrong direction, or if the driver remembers some road construction along that route, he is likely to seek an alternative path. This process of checking current options against one’s goal is a central part of problem solving. In fact, some researchers argue that a crucial strategy for problem solving is a means-end analysis in which one ask over and over as the problem solving proceeds, ‘’how can I use the means now available to me to get closer to my goals?


Hierarchical Organization

Problem solving is not merely goal directed, it is also hierarchical: the effort to solve one problem often creates sub problems, so that one needs to reach certain subgoals on the way towards achieving the main goal, and here too, means-end analysis is helpful:

For example: I want to get to the store. What is the difference between my current state and my goal? One of distance. What changes distance? My auto, my auto won’t work. What is needed to make it work? A new battery…….. In this case, the initial problem (getting to the store) is replaced by a series of sub problems (e.g. getting the car to work). By solving these, one at a time, the large problem gets dealt with.

Working Backward

One useful method for solving problem is to work backward, starting with the goal or final state and seeking a path toward the starting pint.




Finding An Appropriate Analogy

Another suggestion for solving difficult problems is to work by analogy, since many problems are similar to each other. For example: a school counselor is likely to find that the problem he hears about today reminds him of one he heard a few months back, and his experience with the first can help with the second.

Creative thinking

The restructuring of a problem also plays an important role in those special discoveries we consider creative. In general, scholars call the solution to a problem creative if that solution is both new and valuable or useful. Creative is of course evidence in the scientific of Marie Curie, the artistic innovation of Martha Graham, or the literary achievements of Toni Morrison. What leads to those creative achievements? Many factors contribute, but one pattern figure prominently in reports by the creators themselves when they describe how their insight or discoveries arose. In case after case, these accounts indicate that critical insights arrive rather abruptly, typically at unexpected times and place. Often, the thinker has been working steadily on the problem for some tie but making relatively little progress. She then set the problem aside in order to rest or to engage in some other activity. It is during this other activity that the insight emerges. This pattern has often been attributed to a process of incubation. The idea here is that the thinker believes she has set the problem aside but is actually continuing to think about it unconsciously. Some authors have suggested that this unconscious incubation is usually more creative and less constrained than conscious thought, and this is why solution to vexing problems so often appears during the periods when, on the surface, the thinker is paying attention to some altogether different matter.






How To Solve It

Your Ad Here
Problem Of Obesity And Its Treatment


The seriousness of the problem has led to several attempts at remedying it. No year goes by without some faddist dietary regime topping the bestseller list recommending the adoption of eccentric eating habit. However, there are some psychological intervention that have successfully helped the weight management of the obese, although, the drawback of the psychological interventions is that obese people do not maintain their reduction regime.

Interventions are recommended when a person has a serious BMI statistic and a risk of developing ill health. For people with a BMI of below 27kg/m2, clinicians recommend an increase in physical activity and a decrease in fact and sugar intake, coupled self-directed efforts to maintain weight loss and the taking of a doctor’s advice. For people with higher BMIs, the same intervention may be implemented but if this does not work, more dramatic measures may be adopted such as drugs or, for those with a BMI in excess of 40kg/m2, surgery ( this is called bariatric surgery). The side affects of surgery are considerable, but weight loss is produced more efficiently.



Initial success at weight loss depends on the goals of the person losing weight. First, people need to be motivated to lose weight. Secondly, they need to realise that weight reduction programs are designed for health rather than aesthetic reasons. For example, until recently, interventions were guided towards helping people achieve their ideal weight ( rather than a weight which would reduce the risk of ill health), current emphasis, however, is on reducing health complication and so a loss of 5-15% can be effective in producing this reduction, even though the patient/client may not be happy with having lost so little weight and expect a weight loss of 20-35%. One study which reported that weight reduction of 7kg/m2 combined with 150 minutes exercise a week reduced the likelihood of developing diabetes by 58%.


Psychological interventions

Obese people clearly have difficult in restricting their food intake. This difficulty is decreased during holiday periods which the overweight regard as high risk period because of family celebrations or national holiday celebrations. One way in which this difficulty could be relieved is by self monitoring, the act of closely observing what is eaten and when and in which contexts. Boutelle et.al (1999) examined the effect of self monitoring during this high risk holiday period in group of overweight individuals on a weight loss program. Forty-nine obese people who had been on the program for at least a month, were know to self monitor rarely and who were willing to be contacted in a two eek holiday period (Xmas and New Year) took part in the study. Self monitoring took form of examining food intake and taking weight measurements daily.



Two groups (intervention group and non intervention group) were observed and both groups were encouraged to do this basic level of self monitoring. However, the intervention group also received daily mailing from the researchers as well as one or two phone calls in each week in two week period reminding them to self monitor. Those in the intervention group not only self monitored more frequently but also managed their weight more effectively than did those in the non intervention group. Both group had difficult in managing their weight. However, those in the self monitoring group lost more weight and there was a strong associate between self monitoring and weight loss. The difficulty is maintaining weight loss is one of the most serious problems obese people face. Studies of 20 week intervention programs, such as the one described, have found that people can lose up to 9kg( around 9% of body weight) but when the intervention stops, people can regain as much one third of their weight in the following 12 months.

Clinicians, therefore, have focused their attention on how to achieve the maintenance of weight loss. This can be done by creative detailed behavioural plan for the client to follow, as well as by controlling the portions of food that client eats. Replacing normal meals and snacks with portion controlled meals, for example, can lead 8% reduction in weight over a sustained period of dietary regime control. The longer the intervention, the greater the weight loss, but most of the loss is seen in the first six month. The greater predictor of whether people maintain their loss is their ability to engage in physical activity. Those who do, tend to maintain their weight loss programs. Those who exercise at home are more successful at maintenance that is those who attend gyms or leisure center.