Custom Search

Friday 8 August 2008

Alienation Test

Your Ad Here

Early Theories

There are two early cognitive social theories, those of Bandura and Mischel. Bandura pioneered
the study of observational learning (or vicarious conditioning). He believed that, rather than operating in a mechanistic way, reinforcement provides information about future reinforcement. Such information can be gleaned by watching models' behavior rather than by behaving in a particular way and experiencing the consequences oneself. Note how this definition of reinforcement differs from that of Skinner, for whom one had to experience reinforcement personally to increase a target behavior. Note also that for Bandura, thinking is not an irrelevant activity that occurs within a "black box," but rather is an important object of study in its own right.

A number of points distinguish the cognitive social approach from other approaches, including the following.

    Cognitive Focus--Remember Skinner and the "black box"? This approach is nothing like that. For example, memory of past reinforcements is an important variable mediating stimulus and response.

    Social-Interpersonal Focus--Remember Skinner and the generalization from pigeons and rats to all organisms (including humans)? This approach is nothing like that. Instead, the focus is on human behavior in particular situations, and the most important situational variable is other people.

    Belief in Human Freedom and Choice--Remember Skinner and the call to go "beyond freedom and dignity" to scientific understanding and control of human behavior? This approach is nothing like that. Instead, there is a belief in human choice from a number of possible behaviors. The environment does not only influence the person--the person also influences the environment. (This is called reciprocal determination.)

Five kinds of cognitive social learning variables can be distinguished (Mischel, 1993):
    Encoding Strategies: How do you see it?
    Expectancies: What will happen?
    Values: What is it worth? What are your goals?
    Plans: How can you achieve it?
    Competencies: What can you do?

Although these cognitive variables obviously have an impact on social relations, a question of keen interest is whether they can be reduced to general intelligence. For example, are competencies part of the "positive manifold"--that is, are they positively correlated with all other cognitive abilities? If so, then perhaps theorists would be better off focusing on general intelligence rather than on each individual cognitive social learning variable.

Cognition and Emotion

Higgins' Theory

Higgins (1987) proposed that each person has multiple mental representations of the self, and that a discrepancy between any pair of these representations has emotional consequences. The three big categories of representations are actual, ideal, and ought self. The actual self is who one really is. The ideal self is who one would like to be. The ought self is who one feels it is one's duty to be.

The actual, ideal, and ought selves can be further divided according to whether they are held by oneself or by others. For example, there is an actual/own self, an ideal/own self, and an ideal/other self. An actual/own:ideal/own discrepancy (for example) results in dejection, whereas an actual/own:ideal/other discrepancy results in shame.

Higgins' (1987) theory is an elaborate way of subdividing personality. Higgins' division of personality into six components might be compared to Freud's division into three (id, ego, and superego).

Seligman's Theory

Seligman's theory of learned helplessness was originally applied to dogs tested in a shuttle box with a divider separating two sides. Dogs who were shocked on one side eventually jumped over to the other, and, finding that they were not shocked there, learned the jumping response. However, dogs who were initially shocked uncontrollably failed to learn the jumping response, even if they did happen to jump over randomly once or twice.

The phenomenon of learned helplessness bears much in common with depression in humans. The theory was reformulated (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978) in order to take account of explanatory style--that is, the way people explain negative events to themselves. People who have a pessimistic explanatory style explain negative events as stable, global, and internal. Such people are hypothesized to be more predisposed to depression than people with an optimistic explanatory style, who explain negative events as unstable, specific, and external.

The reformulated learned helplessness model (Abramson et al., 1978) bears a striking similarity to the negative cognitive triad in Beck's cognitive theory of depression (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979).

Abramson, Seligman, and Teasdale (1978)

Beck, Rush, Shaw, and Emery (1979)

internal attributionsnegative thoughts about the self
global attributions negative thoughts about the world
stable attributionsnegative thoughts about the future


Alienation Test

"The statements below concern how you feel in emotionally intimate relationships. We are interested in how you generally experience relationships, not just in what is happening in a current relationship.... When you have answered all the questions, the web page will determine your attachment style." By R. Chris Fraley.

Please record how you feel about each sentence as follows:

5 -- I agree strongly
4 -- I agree
3 -- I am neutral, or I don't know
2 -- I disagree
1 -- I disagree strongly

[For a printable, self-scoring version of this test, click here]

1. I don't understand the way people behave nowadays.
2. I don't want what most people seem to want.
3. The future of mankind looks pretty hopeless.
4. Most people act as if the end justifies the means.
5. I don't get much satisfaction from my work (or school work).
6. It's a lonely life for more and more people nowadays.
7. Things don't make much sense to me anymore.
8. My values are different from society's values.
9. There is little room for personal choice anymore.
10. There just aren't any definite rules to live by today.
11. I wish I could feel more involved in my job (or school work).
12. I wish people would be a lot kinder than they are.
13. I feel confused about the world a lot.
14. Most people don't have the same priorities that I do.
15. You can only get ahead if you get some lucky breaks.
16. It seems that right and wrong are pretty ambivalent nowadays.
17. Sometimes I just feel like a robot at work (or school).
18. Sometimes I feel all alone in the world.
19. I don't know what the purpose of life is anymore.
20. I don't identify with my culture's values.
21. There are so many decisions to make that I could just scream.
22. It seems as if you have to play dirty to win.
23. I don't have much opportunity to be creative.
24. I don't get to go out with friends much anymore.
25. Life has become less and less meaningful to me.
26. Everybody seems to have a different idea of success than I have.
27. It is (or would be) scary to be responsible for a child nowadays.
28. It often seems that it's the nice people who lose.
29. It's frustrating if you really care about the quality of your work.
30. I don't see my family as much as I'd like to.




Your Ad Here




5 comments:

Anonymous said...

Depressive disorders make one feel exhausted, worthless, helpless, and hopeless. Such negative thoughts and feelings make some people feel like giving up. It is important to realize that these negative views are part of the depression and typically do not accurately reflect the actual circumstances. http://www.xanax-effects.com/

Anonymous said...

Hi, I'm psychologist. Greets from Italy
www.francescogreco.splinder.com

Saurav [ Promote Bangla] said...

It was nice effort:-) good work keep posting :-)
Thank You,
Saurav Chatterjee
India

Patrick said...
This comment has been removed by the author.
Patrick said...

Its is something awesome, psychology is vast field and it requires many articles like this one to explain.
www.jennifergraf.net